Interference to ongoing activities covaries with the characteristics of an event-based intention. RL Marsh, JL Hicks, GI Cook, JS Hansen, AL Pallos Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 29 (5), 861, 2003 | 324 | 2003 |
Task interference in time-based, event-based, and dual intention prospective memory conditions JL Hicks, RL Marsh, GI Cook Journal of Memory and Language 53 (3), 430-444, 2005 | 252 | 2005 |
On the relationship between effort toward an ongoing task and cue detection in event-based prospective memory. RL Marsh, JL Hicks, GI Cook Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31 (1), 68, 2005 | 212 | 2005 |
Task interference from prospective memories covaries with contextual associations of fulfilling them RL Marsh, JL Hicks, GI Cook Memory & cognition 34, 1037-1045, 2006 | 201 | 2006 |
Associating a time‐based prospective memory task with an expected context can improve or impair intention completion GI Cook, RL Marsh, JL Hicks Applied Cognitive Psychology 19 (3), 345-360, 2005 | 108 | 2005 |
Source monitoring is not always enhanced for valenced material GI Cook, JL Hicks, RL Marsh Memory & Cognition 35, 222-230, 2007 | 83 | 2007 |
Gender and orientation stereotypes bias source-monitoring attributions R Marsh, G Cook, J Hicks Memory 14 (2), 148-160, 2006 | 83 | 2006 |
An observation on the spontaneous noticing of prospective memory event-based cues. JB Knight, JT Meeks, RL Marsh, GI Cook, GA Brewer, JL Hicks Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37 (2), 298, 2011 | 82 | 2011 |
Task interference from event-based intentions RL Marsh, GI Cook, JL Hicks Memory & Cognition 34, 1636-1643, 2006 | 82 | 2006 |
Halo and devil effects demonstrate valenced-based influences on source-monitoring decisions GI Cook, RL Marsh, JL Hicks Consciousness and cognition 12 (2), 257-278, 2003 | 76 | 2003 |
Comparing older and younger adults in an event-based prospective memory paradigm containing an output monitoring component RL Marsh, JL Hicks, GI Cook, CB Mayhorn Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 14 (2), 168-188, 2007 | 70 | 2007 |
Source memory in the absence of successful cued recall. GI Cook, RL Marsh, JL Hicks Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 32 (4), 828, 2006 | 58 | 2006 |
The valence of event-based prospective memory cues or the context in which they occur affects their detection A Clark-Foos, GA Brewer, RL Marsh, JT Meeks, GI Cook The American journal of psychology 122 (1), 89-97, 2009 | 57 | 2009 |
An analysis of prospective memory RL Marsh, GI Cook, JL Hicks Psychology of learning and motivation 46, 115-153, 2006 | 55 | 2006 |
An observation on the role of context variability in free recall. JL Hicks, RL Marsh, GI Cook Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31 (5), 1160, 2005 | 51 | 2005 |
Detecting event-based prospective memory cues occurring within and outside the focus of attention JL Hicks, GI Cook, RL Marsh The American journal of psychology 118 (1), 1-12, 2005 | 45 | 2005 |
Toward an understanding of motivational influences on prospective memory using value-added intentions GI Cook, J Rummel, S Dummel Frontiers in human neuroscience 9, 278, 2015 | 43 | 2015 |
On beginning to understand the role of context in prospective memory. RL Marsh, JL Hicks, GI Cook Taylor & Francis Group/Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2008 | 41 | 2008 |
Retrieval constraints on the front end create differences in recollection on a subsequent test RL Marsh, JT Meeks, GI Cook, A Clark-Foos, JL Hicks, GA Brewer Journal of Memory and Language 61 (3), 470-479, 2009 | 38 | 2009 |
A comparison of activity‐based to event‐based prospective memory GA Brewer, RL Marsh, A Clark‐Foos, JT Meeks, GI Cook, JL Hicks Applied Cognitive Psychology 25 (4), 632-640, 2011 | 37 | 2011 |