Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. CL Folk, RW Remington, JC Johnston Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance 18 (4 …, 1992 | 3383 | 1992 |
Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. CL Folk, R Remington Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance 24 (3), 847, 1998 | 957 | 1998 |
The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. CL Folk, RW Remington, JH Wright Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance 20 (2), 317, 1994 | 758 | 1994 |
Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets RW Remington, JC Johnston, S Yantis Perception & Psychophysics 51 (3), 279-290, 1992 | 584 | 1992 |
Moving attention through visual space. GL Shulman, RW Remington, JP Mclean Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 5 (3), 522, 1979 | 571 | 1979 |
Attention and saccadic eye movements. RW Remington Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 6 (4), 726, 1980 | 482 | 1980 |
Moving attention: Evidence for time-invariant shifts of visual selective attention R Remington, L Pierce Perception & Psychophysics 35, 393-399, 1984 | 357 | 1984 |
Progress toward resolving the attentional capture debate SJ Luck, N Gaspelin, CL Folk, RW Remington, J Theeuwes Visual cognition 29 (1), 1-21, 2021 | 314 | 2021 |
How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? E Ruthruff, M Van Selst, JC Johnston, R Remington Psychological research 70, 125-142, 2006 | 274 | 2006 |
Chronometric evidence for two types of attention JC Johnston, RS McCann, RW Remington Psychological Science 6 (6), 365-369, 1995 | 271 | 1995 |
The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference E Hazeltine, E Ruthruff, RW Remington Cognitive Psychology 52 (4), 291-345, 2006 | 268 | 2006 |
Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. E Ruthruff, RW Remington, JC Johnston Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance 27 (6 …, 2001 | 268 | 2001 |
Is the left hemisphere specialized for speech, language and/or something else? G Papçun, S Krashen, D Terbeek, R Remington, R Harshman The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 55 (2), 319-327, 1974 | 249 | 1974 |
Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: converging evidence from event-related potentials. MC Lien, E Ruthruff, Z Goodin, RW Remington Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 34 (3), 509, 2008 | 224 | 2008 |
Top-down modulation of preattentive processing: Testing the recovery account of contingent capture CL Folk, R Remington Visual Cognition 14 (4-8), 445-465, 2006 | 193 | 2006 |
Modeling information navigation: Implications for information architecture CS Miller, RW Remington Human-computer interaction 19 (3), 225-271, 2004 | 191 | 2004 |
Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings CL Folk, RW Remington Visual Cognition 16 (2-3), 215-231, 2008 | 175 | 2008 |
Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: Has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent? E Ruthruff, JC Johnston, M Van Selst, S Whitsell, R Remington Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 29 (2), 280, 2003 | 173 | 2003 |
Can new objects override attentional control settings? CL Folk, R Remington Perception & Psychophysics 61, 727-739, 1999 | 171 | 1999 |
All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. JL Irons, CL Folk, RW Remington Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 38 (3), 758, 2012 | 169 | 2012 |